Session | ||
31 SES 13 B: Writing and Motivation & Multilingual Children and Literacies
Paper Session
| ||
Presentations | ||
31. LEd – Network on Language and Education
Paper The Relationship Between Czech Students’ Selected Socioeconomic Characteristics and Their English as a Foreign Language Ought-to Self Motivation The Anchoring Center for Educational Research, Faculty of Education, Charles University, Czech Republic Presenting Author:Foreign language (FL) learning motivation among students is an important topic in the European context, as the respect for linguistic diversity has been seen as a key principle of the European Union and learning several languages is necessary for many people (Baïdak et al., 2017). This is especially true for the examination of FL learning motivation among students with different socioeconomic status (SES), as notable discrepancies in the achievement of different SES groups of students have been documented in many European countries (e.g., the differences in PISA 2018 average reading scores between advantaged and disadvantaged students were over 100 points in Belgium, Czech Republic, France, Germany, Hungary, Luxembourg, the Slovak Republic, and Switzerland, the OECD average gap being 89 points; OECD, 2019). Recent review of EFL learning motivation research (Vonkova et al., 2021), however, has shown that the majority of research on EFL learning motivation between years 2016 and 2020 has been conducted in Asia, only a few studies focusing on Europe (10 in Spain and 3 in Sweden out of 90 analyzed studies). Also, there is a scarcity of research on the relationship between FL learning motivation and SES (Iwaniec, 2020). So far, only a few studies have studied this relationship among European students, such as Alejo and Piquer-Píriz (2016) in Spain, Iwaniec (2020) in Poland, and Vonkova et al. (2024) in the Czech Republic. Thus, the link between FL learning motivation and SES among students in Europe merits further investigation. Currently, the L2 motivational self-system (L2MSS) is the most commonly used FL learning theory (Vonkova et al., 2021). The theory has originated in the work of Zoltán Dörnyei from Hungary (Dörnyei, 2009), who distinguished three major motivational components: the ideal L2 self, the ought-to L2 self, and L2 learning experience. The ideal L2 self concerns a desirable self image of the kind of L2 user a learner would like to become, the ought-to L2 self refers to the attributes a learner believes he/she ought to possess to meet expectations of others and to avoid possible negative outcomes, and L2 learning experience covers motives related to the immediate learning environment and experiences, such as the influence of the teacher or the peer group (Dörnyei, 2009; You & Dörnyei, 2016). L2MSS theory is a theoretical framework to L2 learning motivation we have adopted in this study. Our study contributes to filling the gap in the current research regarding our understanding of the relationship between SES and foreign language learning motivation. We build on the findings of Vonkova et al. (2024) and aim to further explore individual ought-to L2 self items and their link to selected students’ SES-related variables. Such an analysis can help enhance our understanding of the ought-to L2 self construct and contribute to future research on the link between L2MSS components and SES in European countries. Specifically, our research question is as follows: What is the link between Czech students’ ought-to L2 self, at the level of individual items, and their selected socioeconomic characteristics (school type, and parental education)? Methodology, Methods, Research Instruments or Sources Used We utilize the data set from Vonkova et al. (2024) collected among Czech students finishing their lower secondary studies (ca. 14 to 15 years old). The students attended either a basic school (in Czech základní škola) or a multi-year grammar school (in Czech víceleté gymnázium), which is a selective type of school with a standardized admission exam. The data were collected at 35 basic schools and 30 grammar schools. We analyze data from 664 students who provided responses to all the variables under study. We administered a slightly modified version of an L2MSS questionnaire from Lamb (2012) containing six items targeting ought-to L2 self rated on a four-point scale (1) disagree, (2) somewhat disagree, (3) somewhat agree, (4) agree. The items were (Vonkova et al., 2024): Ought-to1: I need English to avoid failing my exams. Ought-to2: Young Czechs are obliged to learn English well. Ought-to3: I have to study English well to be a good pupil. Ought-to4: Learning English is necessary because people surrounding me expect me to do so. Ought-to5: Adults who can’t use English may be considered ignorant. Ought-to6: If I fail to learn English I’ll be letting other people down. As for socioeconomic characteristics, we have used the information about whether the student is from a selective grammar school (compared to a basic school). Also, we have examined whether at least one of the student’s (step/foster) parents reached ISCED (International Standard Classification of Education) 6 or a higher level of education according to the ISCED 2011 classification. Conclusions, Expected Outcomes or Findings The preliminary analysis has revealed that being from a selective grammar school (compared to a basic school) is associated with a higher reported agreement with most ought-to L2 self items. Thus, in terms of students’ ought-to L2 self, being from a selective, academically demanding school appears to be a relevant factor. Students from grammar schools, however, seem not to report notably more agreement, for example, with the statement that learning English is necessary because people surrounding them expect them to do so (Ought-to4). Thus, not all parts of ought-to L2 self are related to the school type students’ attend. Having a parent with the educational level ISCED6 or higher is, similarly to attending a grammar school, associated with a higher agreement on most ought-to L2 self items. A large difference seems to be, for example, in the case of item Ought-to5 (Adults who can’t use English may be considered ignorant.). Students with more highly educated parents, however, seem not to report notably more, for example, that they have to study English well to be a good pupil (Ought-to3). Overall, our preliminary findings suggest that ought-to L2 self is related to school type as well as parental education, though these relationships are not consistent across all aspects of ought-to L2 self. A more detailed analysis is required to further explore the patterns of relationship between different aspects of ought-to L2 self and different SES-related characteristics of students. Also, the role of school in the development of FL learning motivation among students with different SES requires further investigation. References Alejo, R., & Piquer-Píriz, A. (2016). Urban vs. rural CLIL: An analysis of input-related variables, motivation and language attainment. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 29(3), 245-262. https://doi.org/10.1080/07908318.2016.1154068 Baïdak, N., Balcon, M. P., & Motiejunaite, A. (2017). Key data on teaching languages at school in Europe. European Education and Culture Executive Agency. https://doi.org/10.2797/828497 Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The L2 motivational self system. In Z. Dörnyei, & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, language identity and the L2 self (pp. 9-42). Multilingual Matters. https://doi.org/10.21832/9781847691293-003 Iwaniec, J. (2020). The effects of parental education level and school location on language learning motivation. The Language Learning Journal, 48(4), 427-441, https://doi.org/10.1080/09571736.2017.1422137 Lamb, M. (2012). A self system perspective on young adolescents’ motivation to learn English in urban and rural settings. Language Learning, 62(4), 997-1023. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00719.x OECD. (2017). PISA 2015 technical report. OECD Publishing. https://www.oecd.org/pisa/data/2015-technical-report/PISA2015_TechRep_Final.pdf OECD. (2019). PISA 2018 results (Volume II): Where all students can succeed. OECD Publishing. https://read.oecd.org/10.1787/b5fd1b8f-en?format=pdf Vonkova, H., Jones, J., Moore, A., Altinkalp, I., & Selcuk, H. (2021). A review of recent research in EFL motivation: Research trends, emerging methodologies, and diversity of researched populations. System, 103, Article 102622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2021.102622 Vonkova, H., Papajoanu, O., & Moore, A. (2024). Foreign language learning motivation and the socioeconomic status of Czech lower secondary students: An analysis of mediating factors. International Journal of Educational Research, 124, Article 102302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2023.102302 You, C., & Dörnyei, Z. (2016). Language learning motivation in China: Results of a large-scale stratified survey. Applied Linguistics, 37(4), 495-519. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amu046 31. LEd – Network on Language and Education
Paper Early Meaning-Makers: Children and Literacy in Multilingual ECE University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg Presenting Author:This paper summarizes the results of my PhD study about young children’s early literacy meaning-making, which I will defend in July 2024. In my work, I adopt a sociocultural framework, that understands literacy as social practice (Barton & Hamilton, 2000). Instead of merely focussing on reading and writing skills, I explore how language and signs are used, which underlying social and cultural norms exist, and how different contexts shape people’s practices. Furthermore, the sociocultural framework highlights the role of language and social interactions in learning (Wells, 2009). When studying young children, this view needs to be extended to include also other semiotic resources, such as embodied modes, or the use of objects (Flewitt, 2005). Hence, early literacy meaning-making can be seen as a process of co-construction, in which participants use language or other semiotic resources, to develop a shared understanding of literacy. For a long time, literacy learning was seen as exclusively reserved to formal school settings. The so-called ‘readiness’ perspective claimed that children have to attend a certain developmental level, before being able to learn about literacy (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). The emergent literacy perspective contrasts this view by acknowledging that children acquire important knowledge and skills about language, reading, and writing before they enter school (Pegorraro Schull et al., 2023). Emergent literacy affirms that children’s learning about literacy begins as early as birth (Clay, 1975) and is driven by their early engagement in literacy activities and their natural interest in learning (Sulzby and Teale,1996). Although this conceptualization has been established in research for many years, the need to develop early literacy practices in educational contexts persists. Studies have shown that children’s early literacy experiences can positively influence their educational outcomes (Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014). However, rich and meaningful literacy practices in early childhood remain rare (Wells Rowe, 2018; Torr, 2019). While literacy in early childhood has been extensively studied from a cognitive perspective, focusing on emergent literacy skills (Brown, 2014), or in the context of shared reading situations at home (Steiner et al., 2021), only a few studies address early literacy from a sociocultural perspective and in the context of educational institutions. Furthermore, studies rarely focus on multilingual children. My PhD study addresses these research gaps by investigating the literacy practices in non-formal early education in Luxembourg. Furthermore, I explore the ways in which children make meaning in early literacy activities and how they employ different semiotic resources. The context of Luxembourg is particularly interesting to address these questions, as recent statistics have shown that more than 65% of the under-four-year old children grow up with at least two languages at home. Moreover, Luxembourg is one of the first countries to implement a multilingual educational policy for early childhood. This policy, called éducation plurlingue, requires early childhood practitioners to foster language development, collaborate with families, and network with other cultural and educational actors (MENJE & SNJ, 2021). Regarding language and literacy practices, this may involve activities in Luxembourgish and French, the inclusion of children’s home languages, and visits to local libraries. Methodology, Methods, Research Instruments or Sources Used My PhD study is embedded in a larger mixed-method project called COMPARE (Collaboration with Parents and Multiliteracies in Early Childhood Education in Luxembourg). This project is co-funded by the National Research Fund of Luxembourg (FNR), the Ministry of Education (MENJE), and the National Youth Service (SNJ). Ethical approval was received by the Ethics Review Panel of the University of Luxembourg in the beginning of 2020. The project COMPARE investigates collaboration between parents and educators as well as multiliteracies in early childhood and care facilities in Luxembourg. Combining quantitative (e.g. surveys) and qualitative (e.g. fieldwork and interviews) methods. Between September 2020 and June 2021, I collected my data as part of the qualitative fieldwork of COMPARE. The data collection took place in three early childhood settings, that were located in different regions of Luxembourg. Using videography and ethnographic fieldnotes, I documented my observations. In each of the settings, I observed three focus children who were between two and four years old at the time of the data collection. Additionally, I conducted semi-structured member-check interviews with some of the educators. The participating settings represent the variety of Luxembourg’s non-formal early childhood sector, as each of them applied different pedagogical approaches, had different organizational structures, and employed different language practices. Furthermore, the nine focus children had diverse language and family backgrounds, which reflects the diversity of young children in Luxembourg. My analysis combines elements of thematic analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006), sociocultural discourse analysis (Mercer, 2010), and conversation analysis (Seedhouse, 2005). Based on an iterative and reflective process, I analyzed multimodal transcripts, vignettes, and ethnographic fieldnotes. Conclusions, Expected Outcomes or Findings My findings illustrate the diversity of children’s early literacy experiences and foster understanding of their learning processes. Firstly, I identified a wide range of early literacy practices that reflect different underlying understandings of literacy and the use of different pedagogical methods and tools. These practices accorded different roles to children and enabled them to express varying degrees of agency. Furthermore, several contextual factors, such as the educators’ view of the child or the organizational structure of the crèche, seemed to influence literacy practices. Secondly, children made meaning of early literacy by using a range of semiotic resources, creatively using literacy tools in play, and through interactions with peers and adults. Children adapted their meaning-making resources according to the situations and the other interlocutors. Finally, the co-construction of meaning involved children and adults making different types of connections between themselves, their surroundings, their funds of knowledge, and literacy. These findings foster our understanding of literacy development in early childhood, by illustrating the complexity of children’s experiences and valorizing their diverse resources. My study contributes to the field of early literacy research by addressing research gaps relating to meaning-making, educational contexts, and the role of multilingualism. Furthermore, my findings indicate implications for policymakers and practitioners. While the multilingual policy for early education in Luxembourg is innovative, its implementation is hindered by a lack of clarity and conciseness. Practitioners need to receive more concrete and theoretically funded guidance in order to develop meaningful and pedagogically relevant early literacy practices. Finally, professional development courses should promote practitioners’ reflective and observational competencies. This could enable them to leverage children’s rich resources to enhance early literacy development. References Barton, D., & Hamilton, M. (2000). Situated Literacies - Reading and Writing in Context. London: Routledge. Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77-101. doi:10.1191/1478088706qp063oa Brown, C. S. (2014). Language and Literacy Development in the Early Years: Foundational Skills that Support Emergent Readers. The Language and Literacy Spectrum, 24. Clay, M. M. (1975). What Did I Write? Exeter, NH: Heinemann. Flewitt, R. (2005). Is every child's voice heard? Researching the different ways 3‐year‐old children communicate and make meaning at home and in a pre‐school playgroup. Early Years, 25(3), 207-222. doi:10.1080/09575140500251558 MENJE, & SNJ. (2021). Cadre de référence national sur l’éducation non formelle des enfants et des jeunes. Luxembourg Mercer, N. (2010). The analysis of classroom talk: methods and methodologies. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(Pt 1), 1-14. doi:10.1348/000709909X479853 Pegorraro Schull, C., La Croix, L., Miller, S. E., Sanders Austin, K., & Kidd, J. K. (2023). Early Childhood Literacy Engaging and Empowering Emergent Readers and Writers - Birth to Age 5: The Virtual Library of Virginia. Seedhouse, P. (2005). Conversation Analysis as Research Methodology. In K. Richards & P. Seedhouse (Eds.), Applying Conversation Analysis. Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan. Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2014). Continuity and Change in the Home Literacy Environment as Predictors of Growth in Vocabulary and Reading. Child Development, 85(4), 1552-1568. SNJ. (2023). La diversité linguistique des jeunes enfants et les pratiques langagières au sein des familles au Luxembourg. Luxembourg: Repères Communication. Steiner, L. M., Hindin, A., & Rizzuto, K. C. (2021). Developing Children's Literacy Learning Through Skillful Parent–Child Shared Book Readings. Early Childhood Education Journal, 50(4), 539-553. doi:10.1007/s10643-021-01170-9 Sulzby, E., & Teale, W. H. (1996). Emergent Literacy. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of Reading Research (pp. 121 - 151). New York: Longman. Torr, J. (2019). Infants’ Experiences of Shared Reading with Their Educators in Early Childhood Education and Care Centres: An Observational Study. Early Childhood Education Journal, 47(5), 519-529. doi:10.1007/s10643-019-00948-2 Wells, G. (2009). The Meaning Makers: Learning to Talk and Talking to Learn (2 ed.). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. Wells Rowe, D. (2018). The Unrealized Promise of Emergent Writing: Reimagining the Way Forward for Early Writing Instruction. Language Arts, 95(4), 229 - 241. Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child Development and Emergent Literacy. Child Development, 69(3), 848 - 872. 31. LEd – Network on Language and Education
Paper Socioeconomic Status and Hope in Writing: Roles of Writing Self-Concept and Teacher Support Lingnan University, Hong Kong S.A.R. (China) Presenting Author:Achievement emotions play a crucial role in students' lives (Marques et al., 2013). These emotions include feelings such as pride, enjoyment, hope, and anxiety that arise from students' academic achievements and failures (Pekrun et al., 2017). Understanding the significance of achievement emotions is essential for educators, parents, and policymakers, as it can lead to the development of effective strategies to enhance students' learning experiences. Not surprisingly, many studies have investigated the association between achievement emotions and students' academic performance, motivation, engagement, and overall well-being (e.g., Luo & Luo, 2022; Mega et al., 2014; Obermeier et al., 2022; Pekrun et al., 2017). However, there has been relatively less attention given to understanding the determinants or antecedents of these emotions. Aligning with the goal of promoting social justice and equity in education, prior research has extensively investigated the role of socioeconomic status (SES) in various student outcomes, including achievement emotions. Due to the limited attention received by achievement emotions in writing compared to reading and mathematics, this study focused specifically on achievement emotions in the context of writing learning and the classroom, recognizing the domain specificity of these emotions (Goetz et al., 2007). To gain a better understanding of the association between SES and achievement emotions, it is crucial to explore the potential mediation mechanisms underlying this relationship. Previous research has indicated positive links between SES and students' self-concept (e.g., Wiederkehr et al., 2015), as well as between self-concept and achievement emotions (e.g., Duggleby et al., 2009). Consequently, it is expected that writing self-concept may serve as a mediator between SES and achievement emotions. However, little is known about whether writing self-concept truly mediates the association between SES and achievement emotions. As certain achievement emotions, such as enjoyment and anxiety, have been extensively studied, this particular investigation focused on the under-researched emotion of hope. Thus, one of the primary objectives of this study is to examine the mediating role of writing self-concept in the relationship between SES and hope in writing. While it is evident that family environment, including SES, influences students' achievement emotions, the strength of this association may vary depending on the school climate and support, such as teacher support. However, little is currently known about the effect of the interaction between SES and teacher support on achievement emotions in writing. Drawing from well-established research demonstrating the relationships between SES and hope (e.g., Raats et al., 2019), as well as between teacher support and hope (e.g., David et al., 2023), it can be hypothesized that teacher support moderates the association between SES and hope in writing. In other words, teacher support may weaken the strength of the association between SES and hope in writing. Given the hypothesis that teacher support moderates the direct association between SES and hope in writing, the question arises as to whether the indirect association, in which the relationship between SES and hope in writing is mediated by writing self-concept, may also vary depending on teacher support. To the best of our knowledge, there have been no studies exploring the moderating role of teacher support in the indirect pathways from SES to hope in writing through writing self-concept. Based on emerging evidence regarding the relationships between SES and self-concept (e.g., Wiederkehr et al., 2015), as well as between teacher support and self-concept (e.g., Mercer et al., 2011), it is possible to hypothesize the existence of a moderation mechanism involved in the indirect association between SES and hope. Therefore, this study aimed to investigate whether teacher support moderates both the direct and indirect associations between SES and hope in writing, mediated by writing self-concept. Methodology, Methods, Research Instruments or Sources Used The study analyzed a total of 1408 students from 52 writing classrooms. Nearly half of the sample consisted of female students (49.6%). The average age of the students was 17.47 years. The variables examined in the study included SES, writing self-concept, teacher support, and hope in writing. All main analyses were conducted using Mplus 8 (Muthen & Muthen, 1998–2018). To address missing data, full-information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML) was utilized, following recommendations by Allison (2012) and Enders (2010). The first part of the analysis focused on investigating the mediating role of writing self-concept in the associations between SES and hope in writing. Initially, the direct effects of SES on students' hope in writing were estimated. Subsequently, writing self-concept was introduced as a mediator to examine the direct effects of SES on hope in writing. The second part of the analysis aimed to explore the moderation effect of teacher support on the relationships between SES, writing self-concept, and hope in writing. A moderated mediation model was estimated, incorporating an interaction term between SES and teacher support. This interaction term was used to examine the effects of SES on hope in writing at different levels of teacher support. If the interaction between SES and teacher support was found to be significant, a simple slope analysis was conducted to assess the conditional direct and indirect effects of SES on students' hope in writing at low (-1 SD) and high (+1 SD) levels of teacher support (Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006). The study calculated 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) for the conditional direct and indirect effects. Conclusions, Expected Outcomes or Findings The present study employed a moderated mediation model to investigate the mechanisms underlying the relationship between SES and students' hope in writing. The results provided evidence supporting the mediating role of writing self-concept in the association between SES and hope in writing. First, in line with previous research showing a positive link between SES and achievement emotions (e.g., Raats et al., 2019), the present study found that SES significantly contributed to students' hope in writing. Second, the study demonstrated that writing sself-concept partially mediated the association between SES and hope in writing. Third, the current study examined the importance of teacher support in relation to students' writing self-concept and achievement emotions. However, the results revealed that teacher support did not moderate the direct effect of SES on students' hope in writing. Teacher support did not moderate the indirect association between SES and hope in writing through writing self-concept. Based on these findings, education stakeholders should consider implementing interventions that promote writing self-concept among students, particularly those from disadvantaged backgrounds. References Allison, P. D. (2012). Handling missing data by maximum likelihood. SAS Global Forum: Statistics and Data Analysis. David, S. R., Wen, D. J., & Goh, E. C. (2023, November). Identifying the Relationship Between Strength of School Social Support and Level of Hope in Children from Low-Income Families. In Child & Youth Care Forum (pp. 1-21). New York: Springer US. Duggleby, W., Cooper, D., & Penz, K. (2009). Hope, self‐efficacy, spiritual well‐being and job satisfaction. Journal of advanced nursing, 65(11), 2376-2385. Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., & Lüdtke, O. (2007). Between-and within-domain relations of students' academic emotions. Journal of educational psychology, 99(4), 715. Luo, Z., & Luo, W. (2022). Discrete achievement emotions as mediators between achievement goals and academic engagement of Singapore students. Educational Psychology, 42(6), 749-766. Marques, S. C., Lopez, S. J., & Mitchell, J. (2013). The role of hope, spirituality and religious practice in adolescents’ life satisfaction: Longitudinal findings. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 251-261. Mega, C., Ronconi, L., & De Beni, R. (2014). What makes a good student? How emotions, self-regulated learning, and motivation contribute to academic achievement. Journal of educational psychology, 106(1), 121-131. Mercer, S. H., Nellis, L. M., Martínez, R. S., & Kirk, M. (2011). Supporting the students most in need: Academic self-efficacy and perceived teacher support in relation to within-year academic growth. Journal of school psychology, 49(3), 323-338. Obermeier, R., Schlesier, J., Meyer, S., & Gläser-Zikuda, M. (2022). Trajectories of scholastic well-being: The effect of achievement emotions and instructional quality in the first year of secondary school (fifth grade). Social Psychology of Education, 25(5), 1051-1070. Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., Marsh, H. W., Murayama, K., & Goetz, T. (2017). Achievement emotions and academic performance: Longitudinal models of reciprocal effects. Child development, 88(5), 1653-1670. Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31(4), 437–448. Raats, C., Adams, S., Savahl, S., Isaacs, S., & Tiliouine, H. (2019). The relationship between hope and life satisfaction among children in low and middle socio-economic status communities in Cape Town, South Africa. Child Indicators Research, 12, 733-746. Wiederkehr, V., Darnon, C., Chazal, S., Guimond, S., & Martinot, D. (2015). From social class to self-efficacy: Internalization of low social status pupils’ school performance. Social Psychology of Education, 18, 769-784. |